Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 April 2022
Sec. Environmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Current Trends in Environmental Psychology, volume I View all 35 articles

Evaluation of Daylight Perception Assessment Methods

Gizem Izmir Tunahan
Gizem Izmir Tunahan1*Hector AltamiranoHector Altamirano1Jemima Unwin TejiJemima Unwin Teji1Cosmin Ticleanu,Cosmin Ticleanu1,2
  • 1UCL Institute for Environmental Design and Engineering, The Bartlett, London, United Kingdom
  • 2BRE (Building Research Establishment), Hertfordshire, United Kingdom

Daylight is an important component in maintaining human health and wellbeing and plays a key role in physiological, psychological, and behavioural regulation. Understanding the complexity of daylight perception is vital since the degree of satisfaction with daylight conditions could greatly impact individual mood, behaviour and cognitive performance. This paper aims at (1) presenting an overview of current knowledge on methods for assessing daylight perception and (2) establishing a methodology for assessing daylight perception in the context of cultural background. An experiment was conducted with 50 students who were instructed to select the best and worst seats, describe the best desks’ daylight conditions and draw boundary lines between perceived daylit and non-daylit spaces in a library. The study showed that subjective rating and seat preference methods were consistent with actual daylight levels. However, participants’ boundary lines did not represent the actual daylight availability in the space. The study suggests that individual daylight perception in the context of cultural background can be assessed using the subjective rating and seat preference methods.

Introduction

The characteristics of an indoor lighting environment could significantly affect the comfort, wellbeing and productivity of building occupants (Al horr et al., 2016). The lighting quality assessment typically includes photometric measurements, which does not provide a complete representation of an environment’s lighting quality (Allan et al., 2019). The assessment should not only consider the links between the lighting levels and the characteristics of the space where light is measured but, more importantly, how people perceive that environment. As of today, far too little attention has been paid to daylight perception and its evaluation methods, as highlighted by the Commission Internationale de l’Eclairage 2013 (CIE 213, 2014) and the Dubois et al. (2016). Understanding its complexity and potential benefits could be crucial, especially in the context of health and wellbeing, mood, and also cognitive and academic performance. Up to now, several studies have shown that exposure to different amounts and characteristics of daylight could enhance students’ cognitive performance (Shishegar and Boubekri, 2016; Jamrozik et al., 2019). However, it is still not known how students’ daylight perceptions and preferences and the level of daylight they are satisfied will contribute to their academic performance.

Culture, one of the essential components of an individual, delineates the characteristics of a group with similarities such as language, religion, tradition, and ethnicity. Knowing the cultural background of a group of people is vital because it could help understand why a group of people acts similarly compared to another group. Lighting research to date has tended to focus on the impact of cultural background on glare discomfort perception rather than daylight perception and satisfaction. Most cross-cultural lighting studies have examined discomfort glare perception and colour temperature preference, but they did not sufficiently focus on the adequacy of illuminance levels. Cross-cultural studies aiming to investigate lighting preferences in interior environments are rare and what is not yet known is the importance of cultural background and its impact on daylight perception, expectation and satisfaction.

In the field of lighting environment, Pierson et al. (2018) have used the term of ‘culture’ as ‘the climatic and indoor conditions to which the subject has been accustomed during the major part of his/her life, his/her behaviour toward this indoor environment, and his/her expectations about it’. Subsequently, a recent study (Izmir Tunahan et al., 2021) has highlighted the importance of cultural background in daylight perception and suggested that the cultural background in the lit environment should be evaluated, considering (1) the ethnicity and/or physiological characteristics of the individual eyes, (2) the area (luminance environment) where people used to live, (3) the luminance environment they were recently exposed to, and (4) the socio-cultural background of individuals.

In the United Kingdom, students constitute 19% of higher education (equals to 438,010 students) with 13.6% of undergraduate, 36.6% of postgraduate (taught) and 43.2% of postgraduate (research) students. They travel mostly from countries with a wide range of daylight conditions that differ from each other and from daylight conditions in the United Kingdom (e.g., China, Malaysia, the United States, Nigeria, India, Germany, France, Italy and Ireland; UKCISA, 2017). Outside daylight conditions refer to the amount and duration of daylight varying with the sun’s position in the sky depending on latitude and atmospheric conditions that depend on various factors (e.g., turbidity, climate and pollution). Hence, students from different parts of the world could be assumed to have previously experienced different lighting environments and students from locations with similar daylight conditions should have comparable daylight expectations. To this end, students’ cultural diversity and the specific lighting environments they were previously accustomed to could affect their perception and expectation towards the outdoor and indoor conditions they found in the United Kingdom (Izmir Tunahan et al., 2021).

Maintaining the students’ satisfaction with the indoor environment they found in the United Kingdom is considerable because the indoor environmental quality is highly associated with the occupants’ health and wellbeing (Sakellaris et al., 2016). The degree of satisfaction, in particular with daylight conditions, greatly impacts individual mood, behaviour and cognitive performance (Wang and Boubekri, 2011). Therefore, gaining a better understanding of students’ daylight perception and expectations could increase their satisfaction with the indoor environment and also cognitive and academic performance. This knowledge can also be utilised by managers and daily operators of university buildings to help reduce the energy consumption of HVAC (Heating, Ventilation, and Air Conditioning) and illumination systems. For instance, a study on Korean office buildings showed that adjusting the indoor lighting conditions based on occupants’ expectations and utilisations helps to reduce lighting energy consumption by up to 43% (Yun et al., 2012). Moreover, it can support architects and lighting professionals working in the design of educational and residential buildings.

In order to maintain the satisfaction and academic performance of the students from different cultural backgrounds in the indoor environment they found in the United Kingdom, we needed to develop a methodology for assessing daylight perception. Therefore, this paper aims to (a) review the methods previously used to assess daylight perception and (b) establish a methodology for assessing daylight perception in the context of cultural background.

The Human Response to Daylight: Evaluation Methods

In order to create a framework of the methodological approach to assess daylight perception in the literature, 482 research articles published in Scopus, Web of Science, and LEUKOS databases were searched for electronic records. The search was done in either title, abstract, or keywords of the papers using the following keywords: (Day)light perception, (Day)light expectation, (Day)light satisfaction, (Day)lighting sensitivity, (Day)lighting tolerance and (Day)light adaptation. The inclusion criteria were: (a) including at least one aspect of (day)lighting perception, (b) published in English, peer-reviewed journals excluding conference proceedings and books, and (c) published during any year from 1990 to November 2021. Relevant articles were classified depending on their methods and reported in Tables 13.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The methods for circadian rhythm related assessment.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The methods for subjective daylight assessment.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The methods for objective measurements.

General Methodological Approach in the Reviewed Studies

Various methods have been developed and used to investigate how lighting conditions are consistent with human perception of daylight and daylight expectations. These methods have been applied in either real-world environments (Keskin, 2019) or laboratories under specified testing conditions (Chamilothori et al., 2016; Chinazzo et al., 2019; Yasukouchi et al., 2019).

Even though real-world environments provide an opportunity to conduct studies in a dynamic social context, people being observed cannot be tested under diverse environmental conditions. Conversely, participants in laboratory settings know they are the subject of study, which may affect their behaviour, making it challenging to associate results with real-life situations (Keskin, 2019). Nevertheless, laboratory studies enable researchers to investigate changes when daylight conditions are changed (Figueiro et al., 2011; Karami et al., 2016), which cannot be tested in real-world environment studies.

Although most methods and tools used in assessing daylight perception differ, their general methodological approach is similar; it combines subjective and objective measurements and assesses them depending on the existing lighting conditions collected by either spot measurements or daylighting simulations. The studies are also often supported by circadian rhythm parameters, such as cognitive performance, alertness, sleep quality, and mood. Nevertheless, almost all studies have used one or more methods to assess the changes occurring in daylight perception concerning the variation in the luminous environment.

Methods Regarding Circadian Regulation

Circadian rhythms are approximately 24-h cycles controlled by an internal master clock in the brain responsible for regulating many physiological (body temperature and hormones) and behavioural (sleep, mood, alertness and performance) changes (Skene and Arendt, 2006). Circadian rhythms are mainly affected by the intensity and timing of light exposure (Arguelles-Prieto et al., 2019) and adjusted at regular intervals by receptors transmitting non-image-forming information of light, which activate the circadian system (Bellia et al., 2011).

Exposure to a high amount of daylight (for example, spending a large amount of time outside or sitting indoors by a big window) has been shown to be related to enhancer effects in students’ cognitive and academic performance (Shishegar and Boubekri, 2016). Previous research that examined the impact of different shading systems on cognitive function performance, satisfaction, and eyestrain in a living lab has also established that satisfaction with indoor daylight conditions could result in higher cognitive performance (Jamrozik et al., 2019). Most researchers have benefitted from commonly used tests and techniques such as the Psychomotor Vigilance Test (PVT), usually used to assess the link between daylight and cognitive performance. Others have also used class attendance or typing speed and accuracy as an indicator of cognitive performance.

On the other hand, several studies have proved that daylight exposure significantly influences occupants’ mood state (Boyce et al., 2003). Küller et al. (2006) indicated that the participants’ mood reached the lowest level when describing the daylight conditions as too insufficient. Specified scales (PSS, PANAS, CES-D and VASs; Figueiro et al., 2011; Choi et al., 2019) and questionnaires (GHQ; Karami et al., 2016) are usually utilised to investigate the association between the exposed daylight conditions and mood states.

Changes in circadian rhythms have also been associated with sleep quality and alertness in addition to mood and cognitive performance (Garbarino et al., 2020). The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) has been mainly used to measure both subjective sleepiness and alertness (Shamsul et al., 2013; Chinazzo et al., 2019). Tools such as the Horne and Ostberg Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire and the Munich Chronotype Questionnaire have also been used to assess the sleep quality of participants grouped according to their sleep–wake behaviour (morningness–eveningness; Jaeggi and Jaeggi, 2011; Adamsson et al., 2018).

Physiological Biomarkers as a Consequence of Exposure to Daylight

Physiological measurements (biomarkers) are regarded as indicators of previous light exposure; in other words, how much a participant was exposed to light during a specific time. The duration, timing and intensity of exposed daylight may affect people’s satisfaction with current daylight conditions and the regulation of their circadian rhythms. Thus, the assessment of physiological biomarkers could play a crucial role in assessing and interpreting an individual’s daylight perception.

The objective measurement of daylight perception considers the assessment of physiological biomarkers such as heart rate (Chamilothori et al., 2019; Chinazzo et al., 2020), skin conductance (Chamilothori, 2019; Chamilothori et al., 2019), core body temperature (Chung, 2009; Chinazzo et al., 2020), cortisol level (Jaeggi and Jaeggi, 2011; Choi et al., 2019), and melatonin secretion (Figueiro et al., 2011; Jaeggi and Jaeggi, 2011). Heart rate, skin conductance, and body temperature have been measured using wristbands, while melatonin secretion is measured using either salivary, blood, or urine samples.

Subjective Assessment of Daylight

Since individuals are physically and psychologically influenced by daylight (Chung, 2009), objective measurements should be complemented with subjective evaluations. However, some studies (Galasiu and Veitch, 2006; Bellia et al., 2017; Lo Verso et al., 2021) have shown that correspondence between exposed daylight conditions and subjective assessment of the occupants is not always observed because of individual differences. Subjective assessment methods mainly use questionnaires to obtain information through semantic differential techniques, measuring the participant’s overall reaction to specific factors such as ambient illumination of different light sources or horizontal illuminance and brightness of a space (Jin et al., 2017; Albertazzi et al., 2018). Similarly, open-ended questions are used to gain deeper and new insights into the feelings towards daylight conditions, for instance, asking how participants describe the lighting conditions and how they feel under those conditions. Information is usually collected concerning the participants’ background (age, gender, work schedule, sleep and wake times, previous daylight exposure etc.), their evaluation of daylight illuminance and distribution, and their general satisfaction with the indoor environment (Levin, 2017).

As a method for assessing previous daylight exposure, questionnaires require participants to estimate the frequency of exposure to daylight in a particular period (Adamsson et al., 2018). For instance, the Munich ChronoType Questionnaire (MCTQ) involves estimating the time spent outdoors on workdays and free days, assuming regular light exposure patterns. Likewise, the Harvard Light Exposure Assessment questionnaire (H-LEA) emphasises the importance of time duration and period of light exposure during the daytime to various artificial and natural light sources. Information about previous daylight exposure is also collected with the use of devices that participants are asked to wear, for example wristbands, Daysimeter and ACM (Chamilothori et al., 2019) before (Figueiro et al., 2011) and/or during the experiment (Rea et al., 2010). The collected data is often supported by self-written logs (Adamsson et al., 2018). These devices are also used to gain insight into the activity and sleep pattern of the participants and the amount of daylight they were exposed to.

Few researchers have preferred other subjective methods such as interviews to test the influence of different daylighting configurations on participants’ daylight perception (Dianat et al., 2013; Gentile et al., 2015). Moreover, the use of evaluation techniques, such as seat selection, have been applied, where it has been assumed that daylight perception and expectation are associated with seat preference and window location (Wang and Boubekri, 2010; Keskin et al., 2017). In this case, the selected desk’s illuminance level could be used as an indicator of daylight perception. Additionally, a unique method was proposed by Reinhart and Weissman (2012) and also used by Handina et al. (2017), given its potential as a representation tool of how daylight composition can be perceived in a space. Handina et al. (2017) have considered the daylight boundary line method to assess perception through the definition of daylit and non-daylit areas drawn by participants. In this methodology, participants have been required to draw a line whenever they notice a boundary between brightness and darkness in the experiment room. Their initial results showed that the percentage of the area enclosed with the contour line of DA300 lx, 50% (illuminance level of at least 300 lux over at least 50% of the space) in the observed space (55%) is close to the partially daylit area (56%), which is the area perceived as bright by at least 25% of participants. Furthermore, high Dynamic image techniques (Jung and Inanici, 2019) and 3D daylight renderings (Rockcastle and Andersen, 2015) have also been used to evaluate the human perception of the daylight composition found in shown scenes. In the further development of these techniques, subjective daylight perception under various computer-generated conditions has been assessed using scenes displayed with the Immersive virtual reality (VR) technique (Chamilothori et al., 2016).

Methodology

Fifty MSc students were brought all together to the Bartlett Library, asked to complete a questionnaire before the experiment and undertake a set of tasks while going around the library. The library was assessed during one of the sunniest days in December 2019 (between 13:00 and 14:00); a day with a clear sky was selected to get maximum daylight throughout the library during the experiment. The day and time of the study were decided based on both the previous years’ daylighting data obtained from Public Health England and weather data from the Met Office. All tasks took between 20 and 25 min to complete. Collected subjective responses from participants were evaluated depending on the daylight availability of the room obtained from a lighting simulation tool.

As previously highlighted, the effect of lighting conditions on human perception and expectations should be investigated using objective measurements and subjective evaluations. However, only subjective evaluation methods with different applications could be utilised to complement each other for situations where a considerable amount of data collection from objective measurements may not be feasible and accessible. Thus, in this study, only these subjective evaluation methods were applied; seat preference, subjective ratings and daylight boundary line drawings.

Participants

An invitation to participate in the study was sent via email to 348 postgraduate students enrolled in MSc programs at the Bartlett School of Architecture, UCL. Seventy-six responded that they would be happy to be involved in the experiment, but only 50 students (15 males/35 females) aged 20–34 years old were recruited for this study.

In terms of cultural background, the ethnicity of participants and the time spent in London were considered. Eleven participants (22%) described themselves as White, whereas 33 students (66%) stated they have an Asian background. Only five participants (10%) defined their ethnicity as other ethnic backgrounds. Most of the students (72%) were overseas students who had spent less than 3 months in London.

Field Site

The study was carried out in the UCL Bartlett library located on the ground floor of a six-storey building. The library comprises three main study areas (Figure 1). The group study area (Room 1) accommodates eight shared desks and four individual cubicles and has two side windows in the north-facing external wall; the library collection area (Room 2) has 12 shared desks and 11 individual desks and several side windows facing north and east orientations; the quiet study room (Room 3) is an open-plan space with a skylight, and 32 shared desks. Details of the rooms and technical properties of the surfaces are illustrated in Appendix 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Plan of the Bartlett Library (The red arrows represent the locations from where the photos on the right side of the figure were taken).

Quantification of Daylight Availability in the Library

Parametric modelling and daylight simulations were used to get information concerning daylight availability at desks in each room at the library. Spot illuminance measurements were also used to calibrate the created model. AutoCAD and Rhino were used to produce 2D and 3D drawings of the library. Then, Grasshopper was used to create parametric modelling for lighting performance analysis with Ladybug and Honeybee plugins.

Previous studies show that computer predictions with simulation methods demonstrate higher accuracy than measurements taken in real-world conditions. The simulation method results involve an acceptable amount of error arising from either unpredictable sky conditions at that moment or the incorrect input parameters in the simulation model. Therefore, it is always more reliable to compare daylight performance predictions obtained from computer simulations with physical measurements taken in the real space. Since it demonstrates how much simulation results correspond to actual daylight conditions. Daylight modelling built-in Radiance was validated against actual illuminance measurements at a specific point, date and time. A strong association between simulation results and actual daylight measurements was found (p < 0.05, R2 = 0.89). In other words, the difference in values between spot measurements and simulation results are negligible, and simulation results represent the real daylight illuminances with an acceptable error range.

Contribution of Electric Light to Total Illuminance

On the day in which the study was performed, students were exposed to electric light in addition to daylight.

The contribution of electric light to total illuminance was investigated by measuring the electric light illuminances in the middle of each desk using a Konica Minolta Illuminance meter T-10A on the 30 November 2019 between 16:45 and 17:15 after sunset. Thereafter, these illuminances were compared with total illuminance measurements taken during the experiment. The electric light illuminance values on the work planes were found highly correlated with the total illuminance measurements (p = 0.001). For this reason, it was assumed that all desks receive the same amount of electric lighting, and therefore variations between them would be due to daylight alone.

Subjective Daylight Assessment Methods

Questionnaire Design

A questionnaire was designed to include the three methods used in this study: seat preference, subjective rating, and daylight boundary line drawings. The questionnaire contained multiple-choice, Likert scale, and open-ended questions and was divided into five sections; the first two sections of the questionnaire were completed by participants before entering the library and considered information regarding (1) demographic; gender, and age, (2) time spent in London (months). The following three sections considered specific questions and tasks related to the methods explored to measure participants’ daylight perception; (3) seating preference and reasons for seat selection, (4) evaluation of daylight availability at the best seat selected, and (5) differentiation between daylit and non-daylit spaces (boundary line drawing). The procedure order was specifically designed to start with open questions regarding seat preference, and after then daylight specific questions to lead on to influence the participants’ responses, thus the latter questions would not impact the responses to the former ones. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the UCL Research Ethics Committee in November 2019.

Task 1: Seat Preference

Seating that meets students’ needs and preferences could promote a longer stay in the libraries and keep students motivated, influencing their emotions and learning abilities. Many disciplines have extensively discussed the influential factors on seat preference in a learning environment. It has been shown that the affecting factors arising from the physical environment that govern the decision of seat selection are daylight (Othman and Mazli, 2012; Keskin et al., 2017), ambient temperature, type of furniture, proximity to other occupants (Dubois et al., 2009), quietness, outdoor view, privacy, social interactions such as close to friends, entrance or circulation (Gou et al., 2018), students’ degree of territoriality and seat arrangements (Kaya and Burgess, 2007).

Even though the importance of daylight on seat preference varies from study to study depending on the function of the room, time interval, time of the day and year (Keskin, 2019), some researchers have proved that daylight is the most important reason for seat selection (Alicia et al., 2019; Izmir Tunahan et al., 2021a,b) and the most frequently chosen as a reason for seat selection (Keskin, 2019). Hence, in this study, it was assumed that seat preference could be used to understand whether participants valued the daylight component. The daylight availability of the selected desk was then considered to be an indicator of the daylight conditions the participant prefers. For this purpose, participants were asked to indicate the three best and the three worst seat locations from the library’s seating plan, and within those categories, the most and least liked. They were also asked to specify the reasons for their selection to examine whether the selected desks (best and worst) coincide with those where daylight levels were high and low, respectively, hence if the daylight component is an influential factor when deciding where to sit.

Task 2: Subjective Ratings

The subjective rating method involves asking participants to describe the daylight conditions on a specific desk surface. This method has been utilised in many lighting studies, and many researchers have found participants’ own perceptual statements compatible with actual daylight conditions. However, subjective evaluations may not represent daylight availability completely because of individual differences in some cases.

This method was applied to determine the degree to which subjective statements represent daylight availability in a space and investigate whether people perceive daylight conditions in line with actual measurements. The possible reasons causing the variation between actual measurements and people’s perceptions could help identify ways to increase occupant satisfaction in the built environment. For this purpose, participants were asked to describe the amount of daylight at the best seat they have selected using a six-option scale derived from the BUS questionnaire (Leena, 2017; from very low to very high; Figure 2). Thus, daylight availability at a specific desk was tested depending on how participants perceived it.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The question regarding subjective ratings.

Task 3: Daylight Boundary Line

This unique method proposed by Handina et al. (2017) was used given its potential to represent how daylight composition can be perceived in a space. For this purpose, participants were instructed to draw on a copy of the library floor plan, ‘daylight boundary lines’, whenever a significant change of contrast was found or a bright area was perceived when moving around the library (Figure 3). The drawn boundary lines were then scanned and overdrawn in AutoCAD to overlay the perceived bright areas, which were assumed to indicate the perception of adequate daylight in this study. Finally, all drawings were superimposed on top of each other and evaluated based on daylight availability at a specific time.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. A few examples of participants’ drawings in response to the question asking them to draw a boundary line between daylit and non-daylit spaces.

Methods of Analysis

All the statistical analyses were conducted using the software package SPSS 20.0. Univariate descriptive statistics (response frequencies, means, and standard deviations) were calculated for each variable. Evaluations of the data obtained from three subjective methods were carried out separately as described below.

Analysis of Seat Preference

Initially, influential reasons for the best and worst seat selections and the importance of daylight in the selections were considered. Secondly, daylight availability at the best seat selected was evaluated using ordinal regression. Lastly, the best and worst seat selections were evaluated on the seating map concerning other influential factors on seat selections apart from the contribution of daylight.

Analysis of Subjective Ratings

Subjective ratings were evaluated based on the perceived daylight conditions towards daylight availability at the best seat selection using an independent-samples t-test.

Analysis of Daylight Boundary Line Drawings

Daylight boundary line drawings were assessed with the methodology created by Handina et al. (2017). Initially, the variation in participants’ perceived bright area was analysed using descriptive statistic methods. Secondly, the statistical quartile concept was used to categorise and visualise the areas agreed by a certain number of participants as bright. Spaces were differentiated as fully daylit (area agreed as bright by at least 75% of the participants), partially daylit (area perceived as bright by at least 25%) and non-daylit (area perceived as bright by less than 25% of participants). Lastly, categorised areas representing the participants’ overall daylight perception were overlapped with daylight availability to investigate if they correspond with each other.

Analysis of Daylight Simulations

Data obtained from seat preference and subjective rating methods were evaluated based on point-in-time climate-based calculations positioned horizontally in the middle of each working desk, which has been found to have a better association with seating behaviour than other daylight metrics for predicting daylight availability in previous studies (Keskin et al., 2015). Daylight boundary line drawings were assessed using DA300lx,50% (50% of the occupied time when the target illuminance of 300 lux on a horizontal plane is met by daylight) because of a more robust association with the daylight composition of space than others (Handina et al., 2017).

Results and Discussion

Seat Preference

Reason for Seat Selection

Participants were instructed to select the three best and the three worst seats and indicate the reasons for their selection in an open-ended question. Each participant stated at least one reason for their seat selection (Table 4). The number of reasons stated for the seat selection was greater than the number of respondents who answered the question. This caused the total response percentages to exceed 100%.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Participants’ responses concerning the reasons for choosing the best (left) and worst (right) seats in the library.

Daylight was the most dominant reason when selecting the most liked desk, followed by privacy, outdoor view and quietness, respectively. These results align with the findings of Dubois et al. (2009); daylight was the most significant reason for seat selection. Keskin (2019) also reported daylight as a highly mentioned reason for seat selection in their experiment. In other respects, indoor conditions such as temperature and air quality were other influential parameters for seat selection. Other reasons mentioned related to specific desk features were wideness, proximity to the circulation route or entrance, enabling to study individually or with friends, being at the corner or the back of the room and access to facilities such as a computer or plug socket. The worst seats were also associated with unsatisfactory daylight conditions; and with distractive noise, lack of or unpleasant outside view and non-private environment.

Although, in general, participants seem to agree on the reasons given when selecting the best and worst seats, there were a few cases where a particular desk was selected as the worst and the best by participants. Although seat preference varied from person to person depending on individual needs and expectancies, the majority of the participants considered it important to have a satisfactory daylighting level, face the least people, and have an outdoor view of greenery.

Daylight Availability at the Best Seats Selected

The daylight availability at the best desks selected by participants showed that 44% of the participants (N = 22) described the amount of daylight on their best desk as very high, 42% (N = 21) stated that the daylight conditions were high, and 6% (N = 3) as above average. In contrast, only 8% characterised the daylight conditions as low or very low. These results support the idea that most people prefer desks with a high amount of daylight, which could be with/without consciousness (Kahneman, 2011). Since the awareness of our behavioural responses to the physical environment is limited, and some of our behaviour is not under our conscious control.

An independent-samples t-test was also carried out to check whether there was a significant difference in daylight illuminance level of the best seats selected between participants who indicated daylight as the reason for their selection and those who did not. The findings showed that people who mentioned daylight as a reason preferred the desks with much higher daylight illuminance levels (468.5 ± 437.1 lx) than those that did not mention daylight as a reason (174.9 ± 183 lx), [t(48) = 2.1, p = 0.052]. It could be explained that daylight availability on a specific desk that meets an occupant’s needs and preferences, namely individual daylight expectation, usually influences seat preference. Therefore, daylight availability of the selected desk could be used as an indicator of an individual’s daylight preference.

Other Influential Factors

Figure 4 presents the seat preference configuration against the library’s daylight availability when the experiment was conducted. The categorisation of lighting levels was done based on the recommended range for library reading rooms (between 300 and 500 lux; Keskin, 2019). It can be seen that most (86%) of the seats selected as the best are located in areas with high illumination, whereas most unpopular desks are located in places with poor or lack of daylight. Interestingly, two desks were regarded as both best and worst by different participants. One of them, located in Room 1, corresponds to an individual cubicle that does not have access to outdoor view or acceptable daylight levels. The desk was selected as the worst seat by a participant because of the deficient daylight level; however, another participant preferred it because the desk was at the corner and more private than others. Another desk, described as both best and worst by five participants, is located near the window and in the corner of Room 2. The desk has a satisfactory level of daylight and outdoor view of greenery, which some participants positively appraised; however, others were negatively affected, given its closeness to an emergency exit and facing the people passing through the circulation route.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Best and worst seat selected by participants against daylight availability.

Desks in Room 3 under the skylight had a high level of daylight when the study was conducted; however, they were not preferred as expected. The desks near the window in Room 2 were more popular than the desks in other rooms. Six participants stated that they do not feel comfortable in the open-plan layout of Room 3, even though it has excellent daylight levels, especially at some desks. They also mentioned that their screens were visible to other students and that even if it was a silent room, it was easy to get distracted due to facing other people. These findings emphasised that seat preference cannot be examined only in relation to daylight, and it should be investigated together with other components reported in the study such as privacy, outdoor view and quietness.

The role of daylight on seat selection may also vary depending on the context, sample characteristics, and the activities participants are requested to undertake. For instance, this study’s results could have been different if the participants were in real need of using the space for their respective studies (e.g., reading and writing for an assignment). In that case, privacy and quietness could have been more important than natural environment components such as temperature, lighting and outdoor view. Therefore, the study design might have affected the participants’ natural environmental attention and evaluation of the space and desks. However, although the importance of daylight varies from study to study, it always remains an essential factor for seat selection.

Subjective Ratings

After selecting the best and worst seats, participants were asked to rate the daylight conditions on the work plane at the seat they had selected as the best in the library. Then, the perceived daylight conditions of the participants were evaluated towards daylight availability at the best seat selection using an independent-samples t-test. Although some individuals described the amount of daylight different from actual measurements, it was assumed that the contribution of daylight to horizontal illuminance on the desk had a significant effect on the subjective assessment of daylight, p = 0.002. The correspondence between subjective ratings and daylight measurements proved that subjective rating is a suitable method for evaluating daylight perception and vice versa. However, even if the difference between the subjective ratings and daylight conditions was minimal, inter-individual differences in perceiving daylight conditions need further investigation.

Daylight Boundary Line

Variation in Perceived Daylight

The library’s indoor daylight conditions were assessed by asking participants to draw a boundary line when they noticed a distinction between daylit and non-daylit spaces. A few examples of participants’ drawings are shown in Figure 3. In this experiment, some participants described the daylight availability in certain areas as very high, whereas others found the daylight in the same areas low or insufficient. The overlapped drawings gathered from all participants are presented in Figure 5. Participants’ average perceived bright area in the library varied from ~16 to ~100 square meters (mean = 40.3, SD = 24.6, N = 50). Perceived daylight conditions varied over an extensive range from person to person regardless of actual daylight measurements. Therefore, aspects that can intervene and cause the discrepancy between actual daylight measurements and participants’ perceptions from drawings deserve further attention.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Daylight boundary line drawings of the participants (left), Comparison of drawings with daylight availability (right).

Daylight Availability and the Overall Perception

In order to categorise and visualise the areas agreed by a certain number of participants as bright, the overall perception of daylight composition within each room was evaluated using the statistical quartile concept. Spaces were differentiated as fully daylit (perceived as bright by at least 75% of participants), partially daylit (perceived as bright by at least 25% of participants), and non-daylit (area perceived as bright by less than 25% of participants; Figure 6). Despite the inter-individual differences in the participants’ perceived daylight conditions from drawings, there are still apparent areas in the centre of rooms 2 and 3 that all participants agreed to be the dimmest and brightest, respectively.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Comparison of the overall daylight perception with percentage of the area enclosed with the contour line of DA300lx,50%.

The participants’ overall daylight perception was overlapped with daylight availability in the library to determine the difference between perceived daylight availability from drawings and actual daylight measurements. Handina et al. (Handina et al., 2017) found that the most compatible metric to evaluate boundary line drawings concerning daylight availability in a space is DA300lx,50%. Thus, Daylight Autonomy (300 lx,50%) was used to evaluate daylight boundary line drawings as a daylight availability metric that corresponds to 50% of the occupied time when the target illuminance of 300 lux on a horizontal plane is met by daylight.

As seen in Figure 6, only in Room 3, the percentage of the area enclosed with the contour line of DA300lx,50% (41.3%) to some extent, corresponds to the partially daylit area (45.1%). However, the percentage of DA300lx,50%, was not close to the other two rooms’ fully daylit areas. Therefore, this method could somewhat explain the tendency in daylight perception of a group of people, despite the noticeable inter-individual differences in the daylight boundary line drawings. It could be useful for comparing daylight perception of a particular group of people, such as people’s perceptions living in different latitudes. However, space’s characteristics such as the room’s size, window type and size, and seat configuration could explain the possible difference in participants perception. Also, as seen in Figure 5, the degree of agreement in the participants’ perceived bright area varied. Even though perceived bright areas varied from person to person in Room 2 and 3, the agreed daylit space was more noticeable. Perceived bright areas in Room 1 varied on a wide range, and there is no agreement in the participants’ perception. These findings agree with Handina et al.’s (Handina et al., 2017) work, where a noticeable difference was found in the subjective daylight evaluations between small and large spaces. Overall, these findings indicate that this method could be used to compare the overall daylight perception of a particular group of people; however, it needs further investigation for the individual assessment of subjective daylight.

Initial Findings From the Developed Methodology

This paper aims to review the methods previously used to assess daylight perception and establish a methodology for assessing daylight perception in the context of cultural background. As mentioned in the results section, seating preference and subjective ratings seem as suitable methods for evaluating daylight perception of individuals. Therefore, as a part of cultural background in the lit environment (Izmir Tunahan et al., 2021), the contribution of ethnic background and time spent in a specific environment to the participants’ responses was analysed. The results from the seat preference method showed that when selecting the best seats, the leading reason for 48.5% of Asian participants was daylight, followed by privacy (15.2%), quietness (6.1%) and indoor conditions (6.1%). On the other hand, 33.4% of White participants selected their favourite desks considering daylight as a priority. Subjective rating method results also showed that Asian participants described daylight conditions on the best-selected desks as equal or lower than actual measurements. In contrast, White participants described daylight conditions as similar or higher than actual daylight conditions. This finding shows similarity with Lee and Kim’s (Lee and Kim, 2007) study, which showed that Asian people felt more comfortable than Caucasians towards high glare levels of luminance.

In terms of time spent in London, study findings showed that participants that had been in London for longer periods gave less weight to daylight while selecting a seat than students that arrived a couple of months before the study. Four students born and grew up in London preferred desks with significantly less daylight than non-Londoners. In parallel with their seating preferences, students who spent more time in London described the daylight conditions at the best desk as more acceptable. Acclimatisation to daylight conditions over time could affect subjective daylight evaluations and explain this finding just as shown by Martin et al. (Hébert et al., 2002). However, participants’ daily routine, how long they are exposed to outdoor daylight conditions and in which timeframe also matter in addition to the daylight availability of the city. Together these findings show that there could be an association between cultural background and subjective daylight evaluations; however, it needs further investigation with a large sample size of participants considering all cultural background components.

Limitations and Future Work

• The study was limited to a particular place and a particular group of people at a given point in time. The small sample size was another limitation that did not allow to generalise of the findings.

• The role of daylight on seat selection may vary depending on the context, sample characteristics, and the activities participants are requested to undertake. Study results could have been different if the participants were in real need of using the space for their respective studies (e.g., reading and writing for an assignment). In that case, privacy and quietness could have been more important than natural environment components such as temperature, lighting and outdoor view. Therefore, the study design might have affected the participants’ natural environmental attention and evaluation of the space and desks.

• Even if the difference between the subjective ratings and daylight conditions was minimal, the reasons for perceiving daylight conditions different from other individuals need further investigation, and inter-individual differences should be examined deeply in further studies.

• The use of drawings to measure participants’ perceptions, such as the daylight boundary line method, has some limitations because it involves cognitive and motor processing simultaneously. Therefore, it is suggested (Mitchell et al., 2011) that when a drawing is used as a research method, it should entail participants’ drawing and talking, or drawing and writing to interpret the meaning embedded in their drawings.

• The impact of cultural background on daylight perception was evaluated considering only ethnic background and time spent in London. However, cultural background in the lit environment comprises many aspects. Further analysis is needed as suggested by (UKCISA, 2017) considering the luminance environment where people used to live, the luminance environment they were recently exposed to, the socio-cultural background, and individual lifestyle daily routines.

Conclusion

Daylighting is an essential component of the indoor environment that can greatly influence the occupants’ comfort and wellbeing. For assessing the daylighting quality, photometric measurements on their own do not wholly represent the subjective aspect of the lighting environment; therefore, more attention should be paid to how participants perceive the same daylight conditions and which method can predict the daylight perception of the participants much better. This paper has presented a summary of current methods for assessing daylight perception and established a methodology for assessing daylight perception in the context of cultural background. In lighting studies, culture represents the many aspects from individuals’ characteristics and the climatic and indoor conditions people have experienced. Hence, people from different cultural backgrounds might have different expectations of the lit environment. This knowledge could be used to investigate how users interact with the building and develop strategies to reduce unnecessary electricity consumption in addition to the contribution to human health and wellbeing.

This paper showed that subjective ratings, the amount of daylight described by participants, coincide with the daylight availability on specific surfaces. However, there remains a slight difference between participants’ statements and actual daylight conditions. The reasons why daylight conditions are perceived differently by participants need further investigation. The findings from the seat preference method showed that daylight was the most dominant reason when selecting the best desks in the library, followed by privacy, outdoor view and quietness, respectively. Although the reasons for seat selection varied, the majority of the participants agreed on particular reasons; satisfactory daylighting level, facing the least people, and a greenery outdoor view. This study also showed that the perceived daylight conditions obtained from the daylight boundary line method varied extensively from person to person regardless of actual daylight measurements. Therefore, aspects that can intervene and cause the discrepancy between actual daylight measurements and participants’ drawings deserve further attention. Initial results from the developed method demonstrated that there could be an association between cultural background and subjective daylight evaluations; however, it needs further investigation with a large sample size of participants considering all cultural background components.

Together these findings showed that subjective rating and seat preference methods could be used to evaluate daylight perception. Although daylight availability corresponds better with subjective statements, collecting participants’ subjective responses would not always be possible, especially in large-scale studies. Therefore, the combination of subjective rating and seat preference methods is suggested as appropriate methods for assessing daylight perception. Future research should also consider the impact of other environmental parameters on seat preference and how they relate to lighting conditions to improve occupant satisfaction. The interaction between any parameter and seating choice should not be examined in isolation; other aspects, such as privacy, outdoor view and quietness, should also be considered. Inter-individual differences in daylight perception are also worth investigating further.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, and further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

GI conducted the research and reported the results. HA (primary Ph.D. supervisor), JU (secondary Ph.D. supervisor), and CT provided feedback. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

This research was produced with the financial assistance of the Ministry of National Education of Turkey. The funder had no role in the design of the study, in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of the data.

References

Adamsson, M., Laike, T., and Morita, T. (2018). Seasonal variation in bright daylight exposure, mood and behavior among a group of office workers in Sweden. J. Circadian Rhythms 16, 1–17. doi: 10.5334/jcr.153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al horr, Y., Arif, M., Katafygiotou, M., Mazroei, A., Kaushik, A., and Elsarrag, E. (2016). Impact of indoor environmental quality on occupant well-being and comfort: a review of the literature. Int. J. Sustain. Built Environ. 5, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ijsbe.2016.03.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albertazzi, L., Canal, L., Chisté, P., Micciolo, R., and Zavagno, D. (2018). Sensual light? Subjective dimensions of ambient illumination. Perception 47, 909–926. doi: 10.1177/0301006618787737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alicia, C., Alicia, C., Simon, S., and Isoardi, G. (2019). Subjective assessments of lighting quality: a measurement review. LEUKOS, 15:2–3, 115-126. doi: 10.1080/15502724.2018.1531017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allan, A. C., Garcia-Hansen, V., Isoardi, G., and Smith, S. S. (2019). Subjective assessments of lighting quality: a measurement review. LEUKOS - J. Illum. Eng. Soc. North Am. 15, 115–126. doi: 10.1080/15502724.2018.1531017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arguelles-Prieto, R., Bonmati-Carrion, M. A., Rol, M. A., and Madrid, J. A. (2019). Determining light intensity, timing and type of visible and circadian light from an ambulatory circadian monitoring device. Front. Physiol. 10:822. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axarli, K., and Meresi, A., (2008). Objective and subjective criteria regarding the effect of sunlight and daylight in classrooms. PLEA 2008 – 25th Conference on Passive and Low Energy Architecture (Geneva, Switzerland).

Google Scholar

Bellia, L., Bisegna, F., and Spada, G. (2011). Lighting in indoor environments: visual and non-visual effects of light sources with different spectral power distributions. Build. Environ. 46, 1984–1992. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2011.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bellia, L., Fragliasso, F., and Stefanizzi, E. (2017). Daylit offices: a comparison between measured parameters assessing light quality and users’ opinions. Build. Environ. 113, 92–106. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.08.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bian, Y., and Luo, T. (2017). Investigation of visual comfort metrics from subjective responses in China: A study in offices with daylight. Build. Environ. 123, 661–671. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.07.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bournas, I., Dubois, M. C., and Laike, T. (2019). Relation between occupant perception of brightness and daylight distribution with key geometric characteristics in multi-family apartments of Malmö, Sweden. J. Phys. Conf. Ser. 1343. doi: 10.1088/1742-6596/1343/1/012161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyce, P., Hunter, C., and Howlett, O. (2003). The benefits of daylight through windows. Light. Research Cent. 1, 1–88.

Google Scholar

Chamilothori, K. (2019). Perceptual effects of daylight patterns in architecture. doi: 10.5075/epfl-thesis9553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamilothori, K., Chinazzo, G., Rodrigues, J., Dan-glauser, E. S., Wienold, J., and Andersen, M. (2018). Perceived interest and heart rate response to façade and daylight patterns in virtual reality. Acad. Neurosci. Archit. 2018, 1–2.

Google Scholar

Chamilothori, K., Chinazzo, G., Rodrigues, J., Dan-Glauser, E. S., Wienold, J., and Andersen, M. (2019). Subjective and physiological responses to façade and sunlight pattern geometry in virtual reality. Build. Environ. 150, 144–155. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2019.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamilothori, K., Wienold, J., and Andersen, M.. (2016). Daylight patterns as a means to influence the spatial ambiance: a preliminary study. 3rd Int. Congr. Ambiances. September 2016. Volos, Greece. 117–122.

Google Scholar

Chamilothori, K., Wienold, J., and Andersen, M., (2018). Façade design and our experience of space: the joint impact of architecture and daylight on human perception and physiological responses. Light Symposium 2018 - Light and Architecture: multi-sensory experience, Stockholm, Sweden.

Google Scholar

Cheung, H. D., and Chung, T. M. (2008). A study on subjective preference to daylit residential indoor environment using conjoint analysis. Build. Environ. 43, 2101–2111. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2007.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chinazzo, G., Chamilothori, K., Wienold, J., and Andersen, M. (2020). Temperature–color interaction: subjective indoor environmental perception and physiological responses in virtual reality. Hum. Factors 63, 474–502. doi: 10.1177/0018720819892383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chinazzo, G., Wienold, J., and Andersen, M. (2019). Cognitive performance evaluation under controlled daylight levels at different indoor temperatures. 877–887. doi: 10.25039/x46.2019.po004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, K., Shin, C., Kim, T., Chung, H. J., and Suk, H. J. (2019). Awakening effects of blue-enriched morning light exposure on university students’ physiological and subjective responses. Sci. Rep. 9:345. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-36791-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, S. (2009). How to measure the circadian rhythm in human being? J. Korean Sleep Res. Soc. 6, 63–68. doi: 10.13078/jksrs09013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

CIE 213 (2014). “Guide to protocols for describing methods,” in International Commission on Illumination (CIE) Datasheets for Standards and Technical Documents. 43.

Google Scholar

Dianat, I., Sedghi, A., Bagherzade, J., Jafarabadi, M. A., and Stedmon, A. W. (2013). Objective and subjective assessments of lighting in a hospital setting: implications for health, safety and performance. Ergonomics 56, 1535–1545. doi: 10.1080/00140139.2013.820845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubois, C., Demers, C., and Potvin, A., (2009). “Daylit spaces and comfortable occupants: A variety of luminous ambiences in support of a diversity of individuals,” PLEA 2009 - Archit. Energy Occupant’s Perspect. Proc. 26th Int. Conf. Passiv. Low Energy Archit., June 2009. Quebec, Canada.

Google Scholar

Dubois, M., Gentile, N., Amorim, C. N. D., Geisler-Moroder, D., Jakobiak, R., Matusiak, B., et al. (2016). Monitoring protocol for lighting and daylighting retrofits: a technical report of subtask D, Case Studies. T50, D3. doi: 10.1177/1477153510393319

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, L., and Torcellini, P., (2002). A literature review of the effects of natural light on building occupants. doi: 10.2172/15000841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiro, M. G., Brons, J. A., Plitnick, B., Donlan, B., Leslie, R. P., and Rea, M. S. (2011). Measuring circadian light and its impact on adolescents. Light. Res. Technol. 43, 201–215. doi: 10.1177/1477153510382853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiro, M. G., Nonaka, S., and Rea, M. S. (2014). Daylight exposure has a positive carryover effect on nighttime performance and subjective sleepiness. Light. Res. Technol. 46, 506–519. doi: 10.1177/1477153513494956

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galasiu, A. D., and Veitch, J. A. (2006). Occupant preferences and satisfaction with the luminous environment and control systems in daylit offices: a literature review. Energ. Buildings 38, 728–742. doi: 10.1016/j.enbuild.2006.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garbarino, S., Lanteri, P., Prada, V., Falkenstein, M., and Sannita, W. G. (2020). Circadian rhythms, sleep, and aging. J. Psychophysiol. 35, 129–138. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentile, N., Dubois, M. C., Osterhaus, W., Stoffer, S., Amorim, C. N. D., Geisler-Moroder, D., et al. (2015). Monitoring protocol to assess the overall performance of lighting and daylighting retrofit projects. Energy Procedia 78, 2681–2686. doi: 10.1016/j.egypro.2015.11.347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gou, Z., Khoshbakht, M., and Mahdoudi, B. (2018). The impact of outdoor views on students’ seat preference in learning environments. Buildings 8:96. doi: 10.3390/buildings8080096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Handina, A., Mukarromah, N., Mangkuto, R. A., and Atmodipoero, R. T. (2017). Prediction of daylight availability in a large hall with multiple facades using computer simulation and subjective perception. Procedia Eng. 170, 313–319. doi: 10.1016/j.proeng.2017.03.037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hébert, M., Martin, S. K., Lee, C., and Eastman, C. I. (2002). The effects of prior light history on the suppression of melatonin by light in humans. J. Pineal Res. 33, 198–203. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-079X.2002.01885.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izmir Tunahan, G., Altamirano, H., and Unwin, J. (2021b). “The impact of daylight availability on seat selection,” in Proceedings of the IES 2021 Annual Conference. Illuminating Engineering (New York, USA: Society (IES)).

Google Scholar

Izmir Tunahan, G., Altamirano, H., and Unwin Teji, J. (2021). Conceptual framework of cultural background in the lit environment,” in Proceedings of the CIE 2021 Conference (International Commission on Illumination), Malaysia, 510–518.

Google Scholar

Izmir Tunahan, G., Altamirano, H., and Unwin Teji, J. (2021a). “The role of daylight in library users’ seat preferences,” in Proceedings of the CIE 2021 Conference (International Commission on Illumination), Malaysia 213–223.

Google Scholar

Jaeggi, S., and Jaeggi, S. M. (2011). Effects of prior light exposure on early evening performance, subjective sleepiness, and hormonal secretion. Behav. Neurosci. 126, 196–203. doi: 10.1037/a0026702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakubiec, J. A., Quek, G., and Srisamranrungruang, T. (2018). Towards subjectivity in annual climate-based daylight metrics. Build. Simul. Optim. Conf. 11–12.

Google Scholar

Jamrozik, A., Clements, N., Hasan, S.S., Zhao, J., Zhang, R., Campanella, C., and Loftness, V.. (2019). Porter, P., Ly, S., Wang, S., and Bauer, B., Access to daylight and view in an office improves cognitive performance and satisfaction and reduces eyestrain: A controlled crossover study. Build. Environ. 165:106379. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2019.106379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jin, H., Li, X., Kang, J., and Kong, Z. (2017). An evaluation of the lighting environment in the public space of shopping centres. Build. Environ. 115, 228–235. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, B., and Inanici, M. (2019). Measuring circadian lighting through high dynamic range photography. Light. Res. Technol. 51(5), 742–763. doi: 10.1177/1477153518792597

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

Google Scholar

Karami, Z., Golmohammadi, R., Heidaripahlavian, A., Poorolajal, J., and Heidarimoghadam, R. (2016). Effect of daylight on melatonin and subjective general health factors in elderly people. Iran. J. Public Health 45, 636–643.

Google Scholar

Kaya, N., and Burgess, B. (2007). Territoriality: seat preferences in different types of classroom arrangements. Environ. Behav. 39, 859–876. doi: 10.1177/0013916506298798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keskin, Z. (2019). Investigating the effect of daylight on seating preferences in an open-plan space: a comparison of methods. Sch. Archit. Univ. Sheff. 381:125084. doi: 10.1016/j.surfcoat.2019.125084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keskin, Z., Chen, Y., and Fotios, S. (2015). Daylight and seating preference in open-plan library spaces. Int. J. Sustain. Light. 1:12. doi: 10.17069/ijsl.2015.12.1.1.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keskin, Z., Chen, Y., and Fotios, S. (2017). Daylight and seating preference In open-plan library spaces. Int. J. Sustain. Light. 17, 12–20. doi: 10.26607/ijsl.v17i0.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J.-J., and Wineman, J.. (2005). “Are windows and views really better?” in A Quantitative Analysis of the Economic and Psychological Value of Views. Troy, NY: Lighting Research Center.

Google Scholar

Küller, R., Ballal, S., Laike, T., Mikellides, B., and Tonello, G. (2006). The impact of light and colour on psychological mood: a cross-cultural study of indoor work environments. Ergonomics 49, 1496–1507. doi: 10.1080/00140130600858142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. S., and Kim, B. S. (2007). Development of the nomo-graph for evaluation on discomfort glare of windows. Sol. Energy 81, 799–808. doi: 10.1016/j.solener.2006.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leena, E. T., (2017). Combating overheating: mixed-mode conditioning for workplace comfort. Build. Res. Inf. 45, 176–194. doi: 10.1080/09613218.2017.1252617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, T., (2017). Daylighting in environmentally certified buildings. Subjective and objective assessment of MKB. Master Thesis in Energy-efficient and Environmental Buildings, Faculty of Engineering, Greenhouse, Malmö, Sweden: Lund University. pp. 1–117.

Google Scholar

Lo Verso, V. R. M., Giuliani, F., Caffaro, F., Basile, F., Peron, F., Dalla Mora, T., et al. (2021). Questionnaires and simulations to assess daylighting in Italian university classrooms for IEQ and energy issues. Energ. Buildings 252:111433. doi: 10.1016/j.enbuild.2021.111433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, C., Theron, L., Stuart, J., Smith, A., and Campbell, Z. (2011). Drawings as research method. Picturing Res., 19–36. doi: 10.1007/978-94-6091-596-3_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Organ, M., and Jantti, M. (1997). Academic library seating: a survey of usage, with implications for space utilisation. Aust. Acad. Res. Libr. 28, 205–216. doi: 10.1080/00048623.1997.10755015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Othman, A. R., Aiera, M., and Mazli, M. (2012). AicE-Bs 2012 Cairo ASIA Pacific International Conference on Environment-Behaviour Studies Influences of Daylighting Satisfaction at Raja Tun Uda Public Library, Shah Alam. Procedia-Social Behav. Sci. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Othman, A. R., and Mazli, M. A. M. (2012). Influences of daylighting towards readers’ satisfaction at Raja Tun Uda public library, Shah Alam. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 68, 244–257. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2012.12.224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierson, C., Wienold, J., and Bodart, M. (2018). Review of factors influencing discomfort glare perception from daylight. LEUKOS - J. Illum. Eng. Soc. North Am. 14, 111–148. doi: 10.1080/15502724.2018.1428617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rea, M. S., Figueiro, M. G., Bierman, A., and Bullough, J. D. (2010). Circadian light. J. Circadian Rhythms 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1186/1740-3391-8-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinhart, C. F., and Weissman, D. A. (2012). The daylit area - correlating architectural student assessments with current and emerging daylight availability metrics. Build. Environ. 50, 155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.buildenv.2011.10.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rockcastle, S., and Andersen, M., (2015). “Human perceptions of daylight composition in architecture: A preliminary study to compare quantitative contrast measures with subjective user assessments in HDR renderings,” in Proceedings of BS2015: 14th Conference of International Building Performance Simulation Association (Hyderabad, India), 7–9, 201.

Google Scholar

Sakellaris, I. A., Saraga, D. E., Mandin, C., Roda, C., Fossati, S., de Kluizenaar, Y., et al. (2016). Perceived indoor environment and occupants’ comfort in European ‘Modern’ office buildings: the OFFICAIR Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 13:444. doi: 10.3390/ijerph13050444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamsul, M. T. B., Nur Sajidah, S., and Ashok, S. (2013). Alertness, visual comfort, subjective preference and task performance assessment under three different Light’s colour temperature among office workers. Adv. Eng. Forum 10, 77–82. doi: 10.4028/www.scientific.net/aef.10.77

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shishegar, N., and Boubekri, M. (2016). Natural light and productivity: analysing the impacts of daylighting on students’ and workers’ health and alertness. 3, 1–6.

Google Scholar

Skene, D. J., and Arendt, J.. (2006). Human circadian rhythms: physiological and therapeutic relevance of light and melatonin. Ann Clin Biochem. 43, 344–353. doi: 10.1258/000456306778520142 (Review Article)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tähkämö, L., Partonen, T., and Pesonen, A. K. (2015). Systematic review of light exposure impact on human circadian rhythm. Chronobiol. Int. 36, 151–170. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1527773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, N., and Boubekri, M. (2010). Investigation of declared seating preference and measured cognitive performance in a sunlit room. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 226–238. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, N., and Boubekri, M. (2011). Design recommendations based on cognitive, mood and preference assessments in a sunlit workspace. Light. Res. Technol. 43, 55–72. doi: 10.1177/1477153510370807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yasukouchi, A., Maeda, T., Hara, K., and Furuune, H. (2019). Non-visual effects of diurnal exposure to an artificial skylight, including nocturnal melatonin suppression. J. Physiol. Anthropol. 38, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s40101-019-0203-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yun, G. Y., Kim, H., and Kim, J. T. (2012). Effects of occupancy and lighting use patterns on lighting energy consumption. Energ. Buildings 46, 152–158. doi: 10.1016/j.enbuild.2011.10.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix 1

www.frontiersin.org

Details of the rooms and technical properties of the surfaces.

Keywords: daylight availability, daylight perception, seat preference, drawing, environmental behaviour, method evaluation

Citation: Izmir Tunahan G, Altamirano H, Unwin Teji J and Ticleanu C (2022) Evaluation of Daylight Perception Assessment Methods. Front. Psychol. 13:805796. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.805796

Received: 31 October 2021; Accepted: 21 March 2022;
Published: 11 April 2022.

Edited by:

Eugenio De Gregorio, Università Link Campus, Italy

Reviewed by:

Federica Caffaro, Roma Tre University, Italy
Liuting Diao, Ningbo University, China

Copyright © 2022 Izmir Tunahan, Altamirano, Unwin Teji and Ticleanu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gizem Izmir Tunahan, gizem.izmir.tunahan.17@ucl.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.